Home > Domestication > Belyaev and the Farm Fox Experiment

Belyaev and the Farm Fox Experiment

Many a child has seen wild animals at the zoo, in films, or even in cartoons and wished they could own their very own cuddly baby lion. However, adults know that owning their very own Simba could spell drastic consequences—and families instead give their love to docile dogs and cats that make far better companions. However, recent research into the domestication of wild foxes shows that even wild animals can be bred over many generations to become docile and mutual human companions.
The interplay between behavioral genes and physiology and morphological development is significant. Because of the way a particular gene is located on a chromosome, genes are often replicated and inherited in tandem—for example, the gene for docility may be hypothetically located next to the gene for a small skull, and thus they may be replicated and transferred to offspring together.
This phenomenon is especially apparent on the experimental farm in Novosibirsk, Siberia, where Russian geneticist Dmitri Belyaev has bred foxes for over 40 years. Essentially, he has turned over 700 foxes into a group of docile, human loving creatures. With these changes in behavior have also come many physical manifestations, including changes in skull shape, reproductive style, responses to sounds, fur coloration, and ear shape. The changes in these foxes also mimic the differences between dogs and wolves, suggesting that an important link lies between the wild hunters and our best friends.

Belyaev’s Hypothesis

Throughout the course of his work on domestication, Belyaev noticed that many domestic animals had similar morphological and physiological changes. There were variations in body size, hair turned wavy or curly, tails were shorter, ears became floppy, and animals lost their seasonal rhythm of reproduction. Belyaev believed that these changes were the result of selection for domestication. A balance of hormones and neurotransmitters regulates an animal’s behavior. Genes in turn regulate this balance. In theory, selecting for an animal’s behavior would give rise to changes in the animal’s physical and behavioral development since these pathways are finely interconnected.
However, in order for Belyaev’s hypothesis to be plausible, there were two stipulations. The first was that domestication had to put an animal under strong selective pressure. In addition, genes had to be responsible in part for an animal’s tamability. In fact, Belyaev’s institute has studied both of these aspects. They discovered that genes are about 35% responsible for the variations in foxes’ defensive response. Also, when the group tried to domesticate other animals such as rats and river otters, a very small number of them contributed genetically to the next generation. This made it clear that the process of domestication must place wild animals under severe selective pressure and extreme stress.

The Experiment

Belyaev began his experiment by taking 30 male foxes and 100 vixens from a commercial fur farm, bypassing the initial steps of domestication since these foxes were already tamer than ones from the wild. The foxes were placed in cages and were allowed timed brief contact with humans and were never trained. From there, foxes were strictly selected for reproduction solely based on tameness. Once a litter was born they would be caged with their mother until they reached about two months of age and were caged only with their littermates, and finally at three months each pup is put into its own cage.
A strict series of tests was developed to evaluate each fox for tameness. Starting at one month old and repeated for six months, the experimenter would offer food from his hand while attempting to stroke and handle the pup. This was tested both when the pup was caged and while moving freely with other pups in a larger enclosure. Once the foxes reach sexual maturity, at around seven months, they are assigned to one of following three classes:

Aggressive foxes demonstrated the following behavior

while tamer, docile foxes behaved as follows

An even higher-scoring category emerged in the sixth generation, foxes in Class IE known as the “domesticated elite” demonstrate the following behavior:

  • Eager to establish human contact
  • Whimper to attract attention
  • Sniff and lick experimenters like dogs

By the tenth generation, 18% of the pups were elite; by the 20th, 35% were elite; today 70-80% are considered elite.

Today, 40 years and 45,000 foxes later, Belyaev’s experiment has achieved a population of 100 foxes that are:

  • Docile
  • Eager to please
  • Competitive for human attention
  • Unmistakably domesticated

Physical Changes

The physical changes in the farm foxes strongly mirrored those found in current domesticated species. However, perhaps the most influential physical change was not an immediately observable trait, but rather a shift in the timing of development. Farm foxes were observed to reach critical sensory development stages earlier than their non-domesticated cousins. However, domesticated foxes reached a critical behavioral landmark much later. As this landmark influenced the pup’s fear response, this combination of developmental mutations effectively lengthened the time a fox pup was able to socialize without fear – that is, the period during which a fox pup could become accustomed to humans. Similar disruptions in developmental timing have been observed at a genetic level in much lower organisms; for example, in C. elegans, mutations in certain “heterochronic” genes influence the duration of larval stages, either accelerating or hindering the development of larval cells into adult cells (Griffiths, 2008). As such genes have been found in vertebrate genomes as well, it seems possible that a mutation in one or more heterochronic genes could be at least partially responsible for the foxes’ lengthened socialization period.

The other observable traits, if they were “linked genes,” may have simply tagged along with these genetic changes in developmental timing by virtue of their physical proximity to the selected-for genes. However, certain traits – such as piebaldness – have been shown to be linked to changes in the development of the nervous system, and as such could result from selection for neurological traits (such as tameness). Melanocytes – the cells responsible for the pigment of our skin – arise early in development from cells of the neural crest. As the cells of the neural crest develop, they fan around the nascent head to form the face, as well as spreading about the body to form the tissues of the nervous and endocrine systems. Some of these traveling cells eventually develop into melanocytes and lodge themselves in the skin, where they produce pigment (Leroi, 2003). As the neural crest is what ultimately develops into the nervous and endocrine systems, a mutation causing tameness could also act to disable melanocytes, preventing them from producing pigment and causing the characteristic white stripe on a domesticated animal’s face.

While the alternative explanations for these changes seem valid on the surface, the authors do a good job of refuting them. In particular, the notion that inbreeding could cause these traits is effectively discredited, as the inbreeding coefficients (a measure of how inbred a population of breeding animals is) are so low that the probability of a trait owing its presence to inbreeding is only 2 to 7 percent. They also argue that the dominant traits they observed could not have been “hidden” in the heterozygosity of parent foxes. While this is true, such traits can actually be hidden due to variable expressivity. In other words, genetic variation at other loci (areas of the genome) could suppress or alter the effects of a gene. This is true of some cases of human polydactyly, in which the trait seems to “skip” a generation despite it being a dominant trait (Griffiths, 2008). While the authors make a valid point using the dominance argument, they seem to oversimplify it.

Such variable expressivity, if it exists in the farm foxes, is arguably a consequence of the genetic system in question being a quantitative trait, controlled by complicated systems known as polygenes. However, the authors address this possibility as well, using the commonalities between the fox mutations and mutations found in other domesticated species as evidence that polygene disruption is not at the heart of the foxes’ physiological changes. Their argument, once again, seems fairly sound. According to the authors, disruptions in the delicate control mechanisms governing the development of the neural and endocrine systems produce the stereotypical changes associated with domestication. Such disruptions are due to selectively breeding for what is essentially a behavioral, not a quantitative, trait. Behavioral traits, unlike quantitative traits, are emergent properties: they arise from complex interactions between the developing nervous and endocrine systems, the rest of the body, and the environment itself. Thus, selecting for “tameness” would produce systematic changes that result in the “domesticated” set of traits. Since mammals (and indeed, many vertebrates) share many things in common between these organ systems, it makes sense that breeding for this one trait would cause similar changes in all of them.

Selection and Development

Beleyaev found a clear link between behavior and physical development. The selection of foxes for docility triggered profound changes in their physical development. For example, the docile foxes opened their eyes at an earlier age (an ontogenetic process) and had floppier ears, which are more characteristic of newborn pups. Additionally, the novel coat colors that were seen in the elite population could also be an indication of the change in timing of embryonic development. The foxes acquired coats with large areas of depigmentation similar to the coats of many breeds of dogs, as seen below.

Another interesting change was that the skull morphology became “feminized”—that is, the skulls of the males became smaller and more similar in proportion to those of females. Finally, and most importantly, the reproductive cycle of female foxes was altered. Domesticated foxes became sexually mature on average a month earlier than wild foxes do, and give birth to slightly larger litters. The mating season also lengthened, and some females even bred out of season, while still others bred twice a year. These changes are extremely significant, because out-of-season mating has never been previously observed in wild foxes.

Lesson Learned

Belyaev and colleagues learned that physical changes can occur earlier in domestication than previously thought because of the stresses of captivity. Some of the most noticeable changes seem to be pedomorphosis, in which juvenile characteristics are retained in the adult form of an organism. Yet many unanswered questions persist. It remains to be seen how much further in the domestication process the foxes can go. They are not quite dogs, but researchers believe that their temperaments point in that direction. The foxes, however, will only realize their potential with closer human contact. With the downturn of the Russian economy, the fox population is dwindling without the financial means to support it. To support its continued research, the Institute of Cytology and Genetics has turned to commercial ventures, such as selling the foxes for pelts or for pets.

Video example of successful domesticated pet fox from the Russian Institute:


Trut, L. (1999). Early Canid Domestication: The Farm-Fox Experiment American Scientist, 87 (2) DOI: 10.1511/1999.2.160

Categories: Domestication
  1. PsychStudent928
    October 12, 2010 at 11:25 pm

    It’s very interesting to get an in-depth look at the domestication of animals…brings to mind the evolutionary processes of human beings, in terms of behavior or physical features.

    Also, take a look: http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=GjqkBcZLwVY
    Fox as a pet. Seems to fall somewhere between Class II and Class I – not too eager to please, but somewhat indifferent. Seems a bit difficult to handle on a leash. Just a thought!

    Really thorough and interesting article, guys.

    • Gordon
      October 13, 2010 at 11:20 am

      Watching this youtube clip almost forces me to consider the ethics of domestication and the fox-farm experiment. Is forcing domestication simply for the sake of companionship morally just? Are we forcing these animals in a situation that contradicts their nature? The fox in the video doesn’t seem to be ecstatic about his situation, and treating lives as just another accessory/fad just seems off to me.

      • Allison
        October 13, 2010 at 8:19 pm

        You make a good point – except that the fox in that video isn’t one of Belyaev’s foxes. From what I could gather from the clip, the man rescued the fox and essentially turned it into a pet (correct me if I’m wrong, the audio on my computer isn’t great). Most likely, this fox would have been one of the Class I animals, but certainly not a class IE – and therein lies the point. Caging a truly “wild” animal (meaning, one that has never been under selection pressure for tameness) is certainly questionable from a moral standpoint. They are not adapted to life with humans; they would most likely be in a state of much higher stress than a domesticated animal, and would likely suffer a poorer quality of life as a result.

        While the conditions imposed by the experimenters do seem harsh compared to what we are used to seeing, remember that the Class III foxes are still far more tame than wild foxes. Wouldn’t setting animals free into an environment to which they would be significantly maladapted be just as questionable as keeping them in this manner? These foxes had already been under selection pressure (in the fur farms) long before Belyaev got to them. If anyone, blame the fur farms that began breeding these foxes to begin with. Besides, the experiment was not started in order to create pets – its purpose was to discern the changes that accompanied domestication, and find out if such changes were genetic. They started to sell the foxes only after they began to lose funding (a better alternative, in my mind, than selling them back to the fur farms).

      • Augustas
        May 11, 2015 at 5:12 am

        I think that you fail to see that being human in nature, we have evolve to be able to change the way the very forces of nature effect us. Therefore it is following the laws of nature that allows us to do so, therefore its the foxes role to comply. such as we don’t argue that a bear has every right to eat fish, we have every right to preform acts that benefit us, without using absurdly inhumane methods that is.

    • Katherine Jimenez
      October 14, 2010 at 6:07 pm

      This video was interesting to watch, it seems the fox still has a lot of its natural instincts intact even though he is domesticated. For me, this raises the question of whether or not we can actually completely domesticate an animal? The fox still seems to want to do somethings on his own, perhaps this should raise some concern as to whether or not the animal might one day lash out on his owner. Perhaps you can take the animal out of the wild, but not the wild out of the animal?

  2. Becky Diebel
    October 13, 2010 at 5:08 pm

    I am curious about what would happen if the same experiments were done with different breeds of animals, especially big cats. Although they aren’t dogs, cats have also been successfully domesticated. Animals like Lions or Tigers seem equally as wild and aggressive as wolves and I wonder if they could become tame in a similar way to the foxes. I also think it could reveal interesting information about genetics and psychology to study the difference between domesticated foxes and domesticated wild cats.

  3. Faatima Seedat
    October 13, 2010 at 8:24 pm

    Since the results showed that not all 100% of the foxes eventually became domesticated I wonder if it’s possible that, in dogs could a mutation result in a wild animal. In this experiment it was the change in chromosomes expression resulted in the tamer foxes, but the system could also work the other way. This could be potentially dangerous. Also could the experiment work backwards? If domesticated dogs/foxes were released in the wild, would the ones that survived be able to reproduce and return back to their original wild behavior of their ancestors? Can this work with other animals, meaning that people would eventually be able to take in lions as pets, or is there a limit on the innate wild behavior of an animal that can successfully be altered? Also the article was really interesting, because his experiment somewhat paralleled the domestication of humans as well.

  4. Yi Meng
    October 13, 2010 at 9:53 pm

    Same as Gordon, I feel kind of worried about domesticating animals just for the sake of companionship. Not only is domesticating forced upon some animals, but also it could enhance the possibility that we can not see as many different characeristics in species anymore, since the physical characteristics that are linked with indocile behavior genes will be selected out. Is it possible that we have already lost trace of certain kinds of dogs because those dogs were indocile?
    While humans are dominating the world, different kinds of species, whether it be docile or indocile kinds, should all exist. Selecting animals for characteristics of pets is kind of inhumane…
    Also, I found it interesting that after a series of selection,”the skull morphology became ‘feminized'”. I guess it is true that female creatures have more docile genes in them.

    • Tanya
      October 14, 2010 at 12:16 am

      Interesting point about the females having more docile genes. When they say “the skull morphology became ‘Feminized'” though, is that a biological aspect, or is that more of an overall stereotype/description of one might expect a domesticated animals’ skull to look like?

      Also, I think, like we said in class today, we have lost a lot of species of animals, especially dogs, because of our artificial selection preferences. I wonder what our pets would look like today if only natural selection was allowed to happen. Would dogs still be “man’s best friend?” If we continue domesticating other animals, what other types of animals can become pets? Apparently domesticated foxes are now for sale? Although this does not necessarily fall under animal cruelty, what says that artificial selection is not animal cruelty?

  5. Anamafi
    October 14, 2010 at 9:27 am

    I think it is really interesting that these types of animals such as fox has the possibility of being domesticated. However, I believe that experimenting beyond this point and extending to other zoo animals such as lions could change the unique characteristics these animals have. I also wondered if after training these foxes or any undomesticated animals to be domesticated, is it possible if their offspring would be naturally domesiticated or human friendly without tranining? I think that these experiments are interesting and could bring positive outcomes or usefullness to humans, but it could reduce the excitement of visiting zoos because we have the choice of having a lion in our home.

  6. Alison Lerner
    October 14, 2010 at 3:24 pm

    I agree with Becky that it would be interesting to do the same experiment on other animals. Would the effects be the same? What would cause some of these differences?
    Additionally, I wonder if there are larger implications for domesticating these wild animals in that it could offset the ecosystem. For example, the wild foxes keeps the rabbit population in check. What types of ecological consequences could result from this domestication? Would it cause overpopulation of some species or extinction of other species?

  7. Odette
    October 14, 2010 at 7:25 pm

    The article seemed conflicting when it described the physical changes in young fox pups because while the experiment claims to be exhibiting pedomorphosis, where juvenile characteristics are retained, the article also says that pups develop earlier than in the wild. The former goes to suggest that the pups are stunted in their growth and/or do not need the physical changes brought about by maturity whereas the latter suggests that the domestic environment is actually better for development than their natural environment. How can it be that both are true? In accordance with that, it also seemed contradictory to say that pedomorphosis is due to the “stresses of captivity” while also saying that female foxes had bigger, earlier, and more frequent litters, insinuating that the foxes felt more secure and comfortable mating in domesticity. Are the foxes more or less stressed in domesticity than in the wild? Or is it merely that some foxes are more stressed (the immature foxes) while others feel safer (the frequently breeding foxes)?

  8. Dennis
    October 14, 2010 at 7:54 pm

    What I am curious about is something that was brought up in the comments and in our class discussion on Wednesday–how does artificial selection by spaying and neutering domesticated dogs affect the gene pool? Would it prove to be a direct reversal of the genes for domestication, or would it select for something else altogether? Would this perhaps change the skull shape and coat also? I wonder what would be selected for in an urban environment like USC’s, and I would think smaller dogs have a natural advantage because they can avoid detection by animal control.

  9. Tatiana
    October 15, 2010 at 11:08 am

    What I couldn’t stop relating this to, was babyness that infants have in order to create a nurturing reaction from caregivers. So these pups slow down in development and heads are shaped round (definitely “cuter”) for instance:
    http://peakyourinterest.files.wordpress.com/2010/09/pup3.jpg?w=300&h=180

    and they also do not have any need to quickly mature since they are treated like pups pretty much their whole life (taken care of by a primary caregiver). The artificial selection seems to be “survival of the cutest” and training foxes a completely unnatural behavior in order to live or breed.

    However, on another note, I do not think it is unethical to raise a fox as a house pet because that certain animal knows no other lifestyle and to them, living with humans is normal. The problem I do have is taking wild foxes and penning those up..
    Humans really do seem selfish for experimenting with a living species for our benefit.

  10. Shawna C.
    October 15, 2010 at 11:16 am

    I think this experiment has a lot of implications for human domestication of different animals. Though it is slightly off the topic of canine cognition, I’ve mostly begun thinking about what it means for an animal to become domesticated. If we assume, for example, that a dog is the epitome of domestication and artificial selection, then how do we describe other animals that respond to humans in a similar way but are not described as “domesticated”? Dolphins and even some whales in captivity have learned to respond to human cues; they are highly intelligent and often enter loving relationships with their trainers. How, then, does this differ from the domestication described in dogs? Why is it that many dolphins are “trained” but dogs are “domesticated”?

    • Elaine
      October 15, 2010 at 5:40 pm

      i think you point out an interesting point because, as you mentioned, dolphins and other sea life that we keep in our aquariums seem to just be as curious about humans and don’t necessarily shy away. I think the difference with marine life and dogs is that dogs are constantly living among humans and are incorporated into our lives, while dolphins and other sea life are usually for our entertainment and for shows. In this way, dolphins do not interact as much. In addition, domestication, in my point of view, is taming but over many generations because you’d be domesticating that species. Humans typically don’t breed dolphins for solely that purpose of producing more “friendly” dolphins. In addition, we usually have dolphins for their talents, and personally, that can be considered an “acquired trait” which does not get passed on.

  11. Edward Osher
    October 15, 2010 at 2:58 pm

    In class we talked about two different theories as to why dogs became domesticated. While this theory lends support for the first theory presented–people captured cute and docile puppies and adopted them into their societies and then the friendly animals reproduced to create dogs. I dont believe that this experiment is sufficient to validate this theory. Belyaev has access to equipment and knowledge that hunter gather communities didn’t have. For example Belyaev has cages, bountiful food, medicine etc. Furthermore he knows about the genes, evolution and most importantly that domestication is even possible.

  12. Christie Kesserwani
    October 15, 2010 at 4:29 pm

    When the article talks about possible reasons why docility is often accompanied by physical changes in the fox, it sites the idea of ‘linked genes’– where the genes coding for more feminine aspects of the fox’s physique, such as a smaller jawbone or floppy ears, are close by to the genes coding for docile traits and qualities, thereby increasing the chance of them being inherited together. It also mentions the fact that the pathways for melanin and aggression are interconnected; therefore, more docile foxes tend to lose their original coat color. While these theories are most certainly plausible, I couldn’t help but think that maybe the feminine traits of the more docile foxes are closely tied to the success of a fox in the wild– that is, the more ‘aggressive’ looking foxes (i.e. straight tail, pointed ears, larger jaw and teeth) tend to do better in terms of hunting and mating than foxes which have effeminate features. Maybe, selectively, foxes with effeminate features are treated like the beta or gamma males in a herd of foxes, whilst more aggressive males are deemed alpha. This would mean that the effeminate foxes are naturally selected for to be less aggressive and more suppressed by their alpha counterparts. If this is so, then perhaps the genes are not linked at all– perhaps the docile foxes which were selected for by the breeders had all the qualities associated with a ‘not-successful’ fox– one which does not look (or act) aggressively. As a result, these foxes, which are artificially selected for, happen to pass down their genes to the next generations, thereby giving the dog-like domesticated fox we see today.

    I guess all I’m saying is, maybe these genes are not necessarily linked. Maybe foxes which happen to inherit mutations in their ears (to make them floppy) or in their coat (to make it less colored) are limited by those mutations; they don’t look like a normal, aggressive fox and are at the bottom of the ‘pecking order’. These foxes are naturally suppressed by the alpha males, and when evaluated in a group, they are the ones to be least aggressive towards the breeder.

  13. Erika Ignacio
    October 15, 2010 at 4:44 pm

    This experiment reminds me of the domestication of other animals such as horses, which are used for various purposes such as racing, herding, and in some places, transportation. Humans have been closely working with horses for the past century, so I was wondering if there are also physical and genetic differences between wild horses and domesticated ones since according to
    http://fubini.swarthmore.edu/~ENVS2/S2007/llam1/horses1.html ,
    the process of domestication is similar to that of wolves and dogs. I think the number of generations it took Belyaev to have 70-80% elite foxes proves that even for animals, nature and nurture play important roles in behavior.

  14. Kylie
    October 15, 2010 at 5:56 pm

    Dmitry Belyaev’s experiment with domesticating foxes seemed to be for the sole purpose of scientific knowledge. Many facts were learned about genetic selection. However, we have to be aware that changing or domesticating a species so much that the result is a loss or extinction of a unique life form might be an undesirable consequence of the scientific investigation. I agree with Alison that we must always take into account the whole picture. . .how does the current research affect the entire ecological system. When conducting animal research, the pros and cons must always be considered carefully.

  15. Candice
    October 15, 2010 at 7:17 pm

    I find it interesting how much control humans have over the environment that surrounds us. We’ve been domesticating animals and even foods for such a long time, and as much as I love foxes, domesticating them just seems wrong. I’m kinda troubled by how the researchers that made these foxes dependent on humans are now using them for profit. Also, their new owners may not be able to care for them, which may lead to a large number of orphaned foxes.

    That issue aside, I guess we have to appreciate these developments from a scientific standpoint. I’m amazed at how much the foxes changed, particularly their mating patterns; mating out of season is definitely a big sign of domestication. Animals were domesticated in the past due to human necessity. These foxes are being domesticated with no foreseeable benefit to humans.

  16. Matt Savoff
    October 15, 2010 at 7:25 pm

    In this experiment, not 100% of the foxes were domesticated, which makes me wonder, would this margin of error just be a typical margin of error that every experiment may have? Or, could there just be some foxes who for some reason or another, cannot be tamed? If that is the case, then I would wonder if there are entire species that would be impossible to tame because of some inherent quality which cannot be rid of.

  17. Cristina Arakelyan
    October 15, 2010 at 7:33 pm

    This article does a great job at demonstrating how foxes physical traits have changed after domestication.ex) the depigmentation, skull shape, etc. I also understand that Belyaev’s experiment was done in order to obtain knowledge but domestication of foxes has gone too out of hand. It was shocking to see that foxes are being kept as pets. It is wrong to cage up wild foxes and new born pups just for experimenting and pet benefits. In the video you could clearly see that the fox did not have enough space in the home to even run around in. Considering the amount of dogs and cats that are abandoned/mistreated other species should not be experimented and domesticated into human companions.

  18. Claire
    October 15, 2010 at 9:15 pm

    I’m interested to know how Belyaev can accurately determine “that genes are about 35% responsible for the variations in foxes’ defensive response.” How did he get that number? What data went into that statistic? I would also like to see if that is consistent with other species of animals, like the otters or rats. If all or most animals have similar genetic components to domesticity, it might make locating the genes for it easier.

  19. john agresti
    January 8, 2016 at 10:35 am

    I think the guy could have spent 40 years doing something else vs fucking around with the natural order. Pinhead.

  1. October 7, 2013 at 3:34 pm

Leave a comment